Skilled Decision-Making...
Lubo Minar on Unsplash

Skilled Decision-Making...

What do we need to know to develop good decision-making for both normal and complex conditions?

Despite the importance, impact and frequency with which we make decisions in our private and professional lives on a daily basis, the actual process of making a decision is surprisingly unknown to us. And that is the case, whether the decisions are made in normal or under complex conditions. Many people just don't know how to do it: making good decisions.

For many people, decision-making is more like a process of guessing or betting on a result, be that what they eat in a restaurant to choosing a job or investing in the stock market. Many even inform themselves, ask the right questions, but when it comes to act of the actual decision, they muddle through, decide somehow, and hope for a good result.

The Black Box

From cognitive science (and here: Dave Snowden), we know that people make decisions through a way that is called “conceptual blending”: Being faced with a decision we scan only about 5%-10% of the information available and then do a first fit pattern match (not a best fit pattern match) with multiple fragmented memories in a subconscious process. This process, where we assemble some knowledge, blend in some memories, – especially the recently activated ones – leads then to a unique course of action, that often defies logic and insight. Since most of the time we are not aware of these processes, we have come to label this black box process “intuition” or “gut feeling”.

When asked about the actual decision-making process, even senior leaders tend to come up with a list of actions rather than a comprehensible decision-making process, that other people could recognize or follow (see: LDMA Decsion Making Assessement). People also often invent a story according to which the decision makes sense afterwards. It is strange that this story depends heavily on the result of the decision: if you were successful, the decision-making process is told very differently than in the case of a failure. Cognitive scientists call this phenomenon of experiencing or describing a decision as good or something that made sense in hindsight “retrospective coherence” – also not really a reliable process.

Most often it is assumed that a positive result of a decision process automatically means that a good process has been used. This is not necessarily the case. It is not uncommon in our society that more significance is given to the result than to the process, since errors in the result are often punished. However, if the process itself is not solid, learning from mistakes will be difficult, intuition will not be trained, and chance, betting and sheer luck will be given greater importance than they are entitled to.

No alt text provided for this image

The best way to make a good decision rests on a solid decision-making process (adapted after Russo & Schoemaker: Winning Decisions):

  • Goal and Framing: the general goal of the decision maker, including how he thinks about the knowledge on which he bases his decision.
  • A realistic approach to gathering information.
  • Organize information and weigh different perspectives.
  • The actual act of deciding.
  • An approach to communicate and implement the decision made.
  • Learn from experience, including a way to measure the effectiveness of a decision so that adjustments can be made

So far so good. Unfortunately, decision-makers face further challenges: Depending on the level of complexity or the degree of uncertainty that provide the context to the decision, these steps not only look different, but are subject to completely different dynamics.

The Cynefin Framework

An excellent decision making support is Dave Snowden’s Cynefin Framework. It helps to differentiate between ordered domains and complex areas (see also Dave Snowden’s article on HBR, A Leaders’s Framework for Decision Making. ). Ordered conditions include anyone who may be complicated, but predictable, with known cause and effect relationships, such as with a car repair, or the construction of an airplane. There are aspects in the predictable domain and those that require different approaches in most decisions. Most decision researchers are in favour of a good mix of the two.

As decision aids under ordered conditions, the following decision-making support can be used:

  • Manuals, procedures, instructions
  • Probability calculation, calculation models and algorithms
  • Analysis and expert knowledge, logic
  • Formal valuation models, scenario planning, some systems thinking approaches

What if it’s complex?

Complex conditions and uncertainty prevail wherever we are dealing with natural conditions or with people and their relationship dynamics instead of algorithms and technical problems. There are so many unknown factors and interrelationships that we cannot assume cause and effect, nor plan or predict results.

As a rule of thumb: If there are several hypotheses for the way forward or the pending decisions that seem to contradict each other partially or completely, we are dealing with a complex context. This means other approaches and methods are required than calculation or logic (if-then propositions). It is stunning how often attempts are made to try and make complex conditions manageable, certain and predictable using purely mechanistic methods, an approach that is surely doomed.

Probe-Sense-Respond

In the complex domain, Snowden advises to test ideas first: “probe – sense – respond”. Ideally, several small test projects can be carried out in parallel and with contradicting basic assumptions. In worst case, we learn something. In the best case, we find a dynamic way forward. Through these parallel ‘safe-to-fail experiments’, different assumptions, ideas and hypothesis can be tested without building the decision-making process on blind spots or too narrow mental models (frames) – if possible, before a lot of money, time and effort is invested in the development and scaling of new approaches.

With this approach decision-makers can avoid blind spots in their own framing or some of the major cognitive biases, such as:

  • the Hawthorne effect: not the new approach led to increased productivity, but the mere fact that it was new
  • the confirmation bias: you only see evidence that confirms what is already assumed, but not that that confirms the opposite
  • keeping the same frame in the face of change of circumstances: Encyclopaedia Britannica almost went bankrupt because they kept the frame of reference “we sell books” despite the emerging new CD-ROM technology. Only in the last moment they switched to the reframe “we sell information”). This is especially true when decisions concern scaling, growth, or innovation.
No alt text provided for this image

While some cognitive biases can actual serve as some kind of heuristics to help us to avoid having to scan all the available information in a brief period of time, avoiding some bigger decision-making blind spots counteracts the automatic first fit pattern match and is especially important with decisions regarding innovation, development and scaling.

What about Intuition?

Intuition is essential for decision-making under complex conditions. Curiously, decision-makers have found that our gut instinct sometimes delivers brilliant results but is usually rather frighteningly mediocre (see “first fit pattern match”). The decisive factor here is probably the training of intuition.

When researchers examined the decision-making processes of successful firefighters, they were initially amazed to find out that these neither followed specified procedures nor seemed to make conscious decisions. More detailed inquiries showed that after years of experience, operations managers “simply knew” what had to be done. More like masters of martial arts, they were able to intuitively recognize even the smallest patterns of fire and smoke, decide and act accordingly (recognition-based decision; see Gary Klein’s research).

The German decision researcher Gerd Gigerenzer explains that experienced experts rely more on their (trained) intuition or simple heuristics than they do on complicated algorithms and calculation models when there is a high degree of uncertainty. Instead, beginners should train their intuition before relying on it.

Use Heuristics

Heuristics are mental strategies or simple rules of thumb that help us to concentrate on the essentials with limited knowledge and time and to ignore the rest to be able to make decisions at all. Heuristics are essential in the face of complexity and uncertainty. The rules should be simpler the more complex the terrain in which we want to make decisions. Harry Markowitz received a Nobel Prize in 1990 for a super complex calculation model for investment strategies. When asked, he explained that he preferred to use a simple rule of thumb for his own investments: distribute your assets evenly across many options (1 / N).

Befriend Complexity

In the end, it is important to develop your own abilities to work effectively with decisions under complex conditions and uncertainty. According to Theo Dawson, it is most likely that good decisions will be made when the complexity of a person’s thinking fits well with the complexity of the challenges in the workplace. The most effective and agile decision-makers rely on additional skills, including the ability to think and communicate clearly, and to learn to understand increasingly complex relationships. This includes the ability to differentiate between complex areas and complex domains and their different requirements for decision making. The recognition of the inevitable complexity and uncertainty of our everyday decision-making requires, not least, the inevitable abandonment of the temptation to want to make everything safe, calculable, and predictable.

Sources:

Dawson, Theo: LDMA – Lectical Decision Making Assessment www.lectica.org

Gigerenzer, Gerd (2014): ?Risk Savvy: How to Make Good Decisions“. Penguin Books;

Gigerenzer, Gerd (2008): ?Gut Feelings: The Intelligence of the Unconscious“, Penguin Books

Klein, Gary: "Streetlights and Shadows – Searching for the Keys to Adaptive Decision Making"; MIT Press, Camebridge, Massachusetts

Russo, J. E. & Schoemaker, P.J.H. (2002): "Winning Decisions: Getting it right the first time". Random House

Snowden, Dave: The Cynefin Framework, www.cognitive-edge.com

Snowden, Dave; & Boone, Mary E (2007): "A Leaders’s Framework for Decision Making". Harvard Business Review.

Snowden, Dave: How leaders shift culture through small actions.

Pictures: Photo by Lubo Minar, on Unsplash, Cynefin? Framework used with permission from Cognitive Edge. For more on #complexitycoaching and #decision-making, see www.ezc.partners, and https://ezc.partners/blog/

This is a nice article, thank you. I recently read about Franklin's Gambit and probably the next step would be to have an evaluation on how often / how easy we justify ourselves the reason behind a decision / predetermined conclusions. There may be a model emerging considering Cynefin and it may worth some exploration.

Very good summary of the key elements and pitfalls of decision making. It’s very true that most people don’t see decision making as a process. And for sure are not conscious about It.

Pete Holliday

Co-founder Strateji | MetaAgility? Master Coach | Organisational Futurist

4 年
回复
Pete Holliday

Co-founder Strateji | MetaAgility? Master Coach | Organisational Futurist

4 年

Anne Caspari great article, I always look forward to your posts.

Pete Holliday

Co-founder Strateji | MetaAgility? Master Coach | Organisational Futurist

4 年

要查看或添加评论,请登录

Anne Caspari的更多文章

  • Collective Meaning-Making on Social Media

    Collective Meaning-Making on Social Media

    The Potential of PONTE results for collective meaning-making In the PONTE project – Bridging Bubbles with Art and Smart…

    1 条评论
  • Different kinds of change: group/team vs collective transformative processes

    Different kinds of change: group/team vs collective transformative processes

    Pitching transformative group processes against or with transformational micro-shifts of larger collectives. This is…

    5 条评论
  • Closer to Real

    Closer to Real

    In organizational development, the dominant discourse is if-then thinking and a deterministic planning culture. If we…

  • Presencing and the Scaling Problem

    Presencing and the Scaling Problem

    If, to induce much needed societal change, we need a given percentage of people to get to a certain developmental…

  • Higher and Higher in Leadership Development

    Higher and Higher in Leadership Development

    escaping the pressure of development I am invited to join a think-tank in Sweden in May. I will meet 19 fantastic…

    2 条评论
  • Company Values - how (not) to work with them

    Company Values - how (not) to work with them

    Companies have become aware of the fact that core values – integrity, trust, fairness – can function as attractors that…

    1 条评论
  • International Women`s Leadership Programme

    International Women`s Leadership Programme

    Dear Friends, dear Colleagues, We would like to bring to your attention to an innovative transformative leadership…

  • The problem with change frameworks

    The problem with change frameworks

    I recently wrote about this big feeling of relief and elation when coming across a meta-theory that delivers such a…

    12 条评论
  • Beyond developmental biases

    Beyond developmental biases

    Emergent Themes in Organizational Life: 3 day workshop with Bonnitta Roy, There are many new initiatives and projects…

  • Pattern based intelligences

    Pattern based intelligences

    As humans we are pattern based intelligences. This also implies (paraphrasing Dave Snowden again) that the energy cost…

    4 条评论

社区洞察

其他会员也浏览了