RESTIVO'S SOCIAL BRAIN MODEL

RESTIVO SOCIAL BRAIN MODEL


UPDATED AND CORRECTED EXPLANATORY NOTES FOR MY SOCIAL BRAIN MODEL AS PUBLISHED IN EINSTEIN'S BRAIN (PALGRAVE, 2020) & AT salrestivo.org. THE MODEL GRAPHIC CAN BE FOUND ON MY WEBSITE AND IN THE EINSTEIN'S BRAIN BOOK

The original version of this model was designed with Sabrina Weiss. I have taken it through a number of revisions designed to keep pace with developments in neuroscience and in social neuroscience/sociology of the brain. It was designed to graphically represent and expand Clifford Geertz’s argument for the synchronic emergence of an expanded forebrain among the primates, complex social organizations, and at least among the post-Australopithecines tool savvy humans, institutional cultural patterns. This recommends against treating biological, social, and cultural parameters as serially related in a causal nexus. Rather, these levels should be viewed as reciprocally intertwined and conjointly causal. The claim in a nutshell is that human behavioral repertoires emerge from the complex parallel and recursive interactions of genes, neurons, neural nets, organs, biomes, the brain and central nervous system, other elements of the body’s systems and subsystems down to the molecular level (see Candace Pert, Molecules of Emotion, 1999) and our social interactions in their ecological and umwelt contexts. This implies that we need to re-think socialization. It is a process that simultaneously informs and variably integrates the biological self, the neurological, self, and the social self to construct personality and character. In addition, each element in the model must be understood as a dialectical entity containing its own internal “seeds” of change, and as following a temporal dynamic that may be at different times synchronous or dyssynchronous relative to other elements. Each element is conceived as an information system with all systems multiply inter-linked by the circulation of information. In the latest revision of my model I have added diagonals with double-headed arrows that criss-cross the model. This maps the chaotic dynamics and cooperative neural mass discussed by C.A. Skarda and W.J. Freeman (1990) in “Chaos and the new science of the brain,” Chapter in Shaw G (ed.) Concepts in Neuroscience 2 275-285; and see C.A. Skarda and W.J. Freeman (1987), “How brains make chaos in order to make sense of the world,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences (1987) 10, 161-195. The unit model is activated in a triad of unit models and it is that triad that is the basic model of brain/mind/culture/world. This reflects the idea that the triad is the basic unit of social life (Restivo, Weiss, and Stingl, 2014: 104n1). This diagram is the General Connectome. A connectome maps the elements and interconnections in a network. The term has been used specifically in connection with mapping the neural connections in the brain. Connectomes may range in scale from maps of parts of the nervous system to a map of all of the neural interactions in the brain. Partial connectomes have been constructed of the retina and primary visual cortex of the mouse. In line with these developments, my model represents the highest level of the connectome, a connectome of connectomes.

Based on ideas in my recent writings on the brain (2018, 2020) I can now offer an initial concept formula for the probability of an “innovative thought”. iT_p = qc^2 x K+G, where qc^2 is the amount of cultural capital the person commands and K is a constant that represents the cultural context and network structure the person is embedded in; qc^2 because doubling the amount of cultural capital, for example, quadruples its impact factor.  K=C+Nt. C = Cultural Context, an index that takes into account a variety of demographic, class, gender, and institutional diversity indicators; N = the density and diversity of the network structure of the society. G=the genius cluster quotient at time t. When considering the etiology of behaviors that are traditionally considered genetically grounded, it is now important to recognize that the brain, like humans, arrives on the evolutionary stage always, already, and everywhere, social. Therefore, what we have considered to be linearly transmitted genetic phenomena must now be viewed in the context of a brain that is at no stage of development separated from the social and cultural imperatives that form us. The very notions of “genes” and “genetic” must now be revised in the context of the social brain paradigm.

The next stage in this project is to embed the basic triad of the General Connectome in the nested networks of the social and cultural connectomes locally, regionally, and globally so that we now visualize a Global Connectome driven by the circulation of information across nested networks. On the rationale for a global connectome (my interpretation), see P. Khanna, Connectography: Mapping the Future of Global Civilization ( New York: Randon House, 2016).





Kaia Raine

Master Social Worker | MS LMSW, Mental Health Treatment, Trainer

3 年

Though it may see further development, as it the social brain model has many applications in developing and new model of "mental" health. Back in 2013, there was a lot of controversy because the National Institute of Mental Health, due in part to findings of neuroscience, was ditching the "Bible" of American Psychiatry, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Vol. 5. as a research tool It may still be used as a clinical tool, but diagnoses are often made by multiple psychiatrists, psychologists, and social workers, giving rise to some set of people getting multiple diagnoses over time. Yet, labeling tends to follow a person across time if there is more than one treatment provider. In any case, as research moves on, the social brain model will can be tested and at least inform interventions of those with biopsychosocial problems.

回复

要查看或添加评论,请登录

Sal Restivo的更多文章

社区洞察

其他会员也浏览了